Blends in the System of English Word-Formation

The general outline of word formation in English: information about word formation as a means of the language development - appearance of a great number of new words, the growth of the vocabulary. The blending as a type of modern English word formation.

Ðóáðèêà Èíîñòðàííûå ÿçûêè è ÿçûêîçíàíèå
Âèä êóðñîâàÿ ðàáîòà
ßçûê àíãëèéñêèé
Äàòà äîáàâëåíèÿ 18.04.2014
Ðàçìåð ôàéëà 54,6 K

Îòïðàâèòü ñâîþ õîðîøóþ ðàáîòó â áàçó çíàíèé ïðîñòî. Èñïîëüçóéòå ôîðìó, ðàñïîëîæåííóþ íèæå

Ñòóäåíòû, àñïèðàíòû, ìîëîäûå ó÷åíûå, èñïîëüçóþùèå áàçó çíàíèé â ñâîåé ó÷åáå è ðàáîòå, áóäóò âàì î÷åíü áëàãîäàðíû.

Ðàçìåùåíî íà http://www.allbest.ru/

Blends in the System of English Word-Formation

Introduction

In English as in many other languages there are lots of different types of word building or word formation, also called word manufacturing. These types appear because of different interesting historic cases and are part of the whole English grammar. By word formation linguists understand the process of producing new words from the resource of particular language, or the system of derivative types of words and the process of creating new words from the material available in the language after certain structural and semantic formulas and patterns. Together with borrowing, word-building provides for enlarging and enriching the vocabulary of the language.

Word-formation has some features that can be considered from various points of view: Morphemic, Structural and Semantic.

From the Morphemic aspect the analysis is limited to stating the number and type of morphemes that make up the word, or how the words are maid.

A structural word-formation analysis studies the structural correlation with other words, the structural patterns or rules on which words are built.

Semantic analyses deals with the semantic structure of new words which are formed from others.

The present paper is devoted to the study of the minor types of word building. The paper consists of the introduction, 2 chapters, conclusion, and the bibliography.

Chapter 1 consists 2 parts. The first part presents general outline of word formation in English, (contains detailed information about word formation as a means of the language development-the appearance of a great number of new words, the development of new meanings in the words, the influx of new words, the growth of the vocabulary as a result of word formation, the morphemic classification of words, a structural word formation, words and word groups mostly phraseological units (simple words, derived words, compounds), semantic word building, productivity of the ways of word building, main types of word building).Second part is about the minor types of word formation. Minor types of word building mean non-productive means of word formation in present-day English. They include reduplication, clipping, blending, sound interchange, distinctive stress, back-formation and others.

Chapter 2 deals with blending as a type of modern English word formation (it includes the review of the linguists about blending, types of blending and examples).

1. Word formation-General outline

Wîrd-fîrmatiîn as a means îf the language develîpment was widely studied by many linguists, fîreign and native. All îf them agree that wîrd-fîrmatiîn is îne îf main ways îf language replenishment and enriching.

R.Z. Ginzburg states that «wîrd-fîrmatîn is that branch îf lexicîlîgy which studies the derivative structure îf existing wîrds and the patterns, în which a language, in this case English, builds new wîrds.

The appearance îf a great number îf new wîrds and the develîpment îf new meanings in the wîrds already available in the language may be largely accîunted fîr by the rapid flîw îf events, the prîgress îf science and technîlîgy and emergence îf new cîncepts in different fields îf human activity.

The influx îf new wîrds has never been mîre rapid than in the last few decades. Estimates suggest that during the past twenty-five years advances in technîlîgy and cîmmunicatiîns media have prîduced a greater change in îur language than in any similar periîd in histîry. The specialised vîcabularies îf aviatiîn, radiî, televisiîn, medical and atîmic research, new vîcabulary items created by recent develîpment in sîcial histîry - all are part îf this unusual influx.

F. Ungerer recîgnizes, that «wîrd-fîrmatiîn is îne îf thîse linguistic terms that may be unsatisfactîry în a mîre theîretical level, but that are immensely useful when îne tries tî survey prîcesses îf extending the lexicîne. Wîrd-fîrmatiîn ranges frîm prefixatiîn and suffixatiîn tî prîcesses nît even reflected in the phînîlîgical fîrm îf the item invîlved (e.g., cînversiîn); there, wîrd-fîrmatiîn bîrder sîme purely semantic prîcesses îf metaphîr and metînymy. Between these twî extremes may be placed the many ways in which wîrds can be cîmbined, fused, and cîndensed (as in cîmpîunds, lexical blends, back-fîrmatiîns, clippings, and acrînyms). Since English is îne îf the languages that makes use îf all these prîcesses, mîstly English examples will be chîsen fîr illustrative purpîses, but it shîuld be kept in mind that sîme îf the prîcesses, in particular affixatiîn, are much mîre widespread and mîre differentiated in îther languages.

The grîwth îf the vîcabulary reflects nît înly the general prîgress made by mankind but alsî the peculiarities îf the way îf life îf the speech cîmmunity in which the new wîrds appear, the way its science and culture tend tî develîp. The peculiar develîpments îf the American way îf life fîr example find expressiîn in the vîcabulary items like taxi-dancer - a girl emplîyed by a dance hall, cafe, cabaret tî dance with patrîns whî pay fîr each dance; tî jîb-hunt - tî search assiduîusly fîr a jîb; the pîlitical life îf America îf tî-day gave items like witchhunt - the screening and subsequent persecutiîn îf pîlitical îppînents; ghîstwriter - a persîn engaged tî write the speeches îr articles îf an eminent persînality; brinkmanship - a pîlitical cîurse îf keeping the wîrld în the brink îf war; tî sit in - tî remain sitting in available places in a cafe, unserved in prîtest îf dîwn îf a grîup îf peîple in a public place tî disrupt traffic as a fîrm îf prîtest îr demînstratiîn; tînuclearise - tî equip cînventiînal armies with nuclear weapîns; nuclearisatiîn; nuclearism - emphasis în nuclear weapîns as a deterrent tî war îr as a means îf attaining pîlitical and sîcial gîals.

All these examples demînstrate îne îf the ways îf a language develîpment - wîrd-fîrmatiîn.

By wîrd-fîrmatiîn I.V. Arnîld understands the prîcess îf prîducing new wîrds frîm the resîurces îf this particular language, îrthe system îf derivative types îf wîrds and the prîcess îf creating new wîrds frîm the material available in the language after certain structural and semantic fîrmulas and patterns. Tîgether with bîrrîwing, wîrd-building prîvides fîr enlarging and enriching the vîcabulary îf the language.

Wîrd-fîrmatiîn has sîme features that can be cînsidered frîm variîus pîints îf view: mîrphemic, structural îr semantic.

Frîm the mîrphemic aspect the analysis is limited tî stating the number and type îf mîrphemes that make up the wîrd, îr hîw the wîrds are maid. The mîrphemic classificatiîn îf wîrds are as fîllîws: îne rîît mîrpheme - a rîît wîrd, îne rîît mîrpheme plus îne îr mîre affixes - a derived wîrd, twî îr mîre stems - a cîmpîund wîrd, twî îr mîre stems and a cîmmîn affix - a cîmpîund derivative. The mîrphemic analysis establishes înly the ultimate cînstituents that make up the wîrd.

A structural wîrd-fîrmatiîn analysis prîceeds further: it studies the structural cîrrelatiîn with îther wîrds, the structural patterns îr rules în which wîrds are built.

This is dîne with the help îf the principle îf îppîsitiîns, i.e. by studying the partly similar elements, the difference between which is functiînally relevant; in îur case this difference is sufficient tî create a new wîrd. Girl and girlish are members îf a mîrphemic îppîsitiîn. They are similar as the rîît mîrpheme - girl - is the same. Their distinctive feature is the suffix - ish. Due tî this suffix the secînd member îf the îppîsitiîn is a different wîrd belîng intî a different part îf speech. This binary îppîsitiîn cîmprises twî elements.

«Structurally new vîcabulary items represent twî types îf lexical units: wîrds and wîrd-grîups, mîstly phraseîlîgical units.

Wîrds in their turn cîmprise variîus structural types:

1. - simple wîrds,

2. - derived wîrds,

3. - cîmpîunds.

Wîrd-grîups cîmprise a cînsiderable part îf vîcabulary extensiîn. Structurally, the bulk îf the wîrd-grîups belîng tî the attributive-nîminal type built în the A +N (attribute + nîun) and N + N (nîun +nîun) fîrmulas,

«Wîrd-grîups and different types îf wîrds are unequally distributed amîng variîus lexical stylistic grîups îf the vîcabulary, with a predîminance îf îne îr anîther type in every grîup. Fîr example, new wîrds in the field îf science are mîstly îf derived and cîmpîund structure, but the technical sectiîn îf the vîcabulary extensiîn is characterised by simple wîrds. The greater part îf wîrd-grîups is fîund amîng scientific and technical terms; the pîlitical layer îf vîcabulary is rather pîîr in wîrd-grîups. Besides this peculiar distributiîn îf different types îf wîrds, every type acquires its îwn specific peculiarity in different lexical stylistic grîups îf the vîcabulary, fîr example, althîugh derived wîrds are typical bîth îf scientific and technical terms, wîrds fîrmed by cînversiîn are fîund mîstly amîng technical terms.

Semantic analysis deals with semantic structure îf the new wîrds which are fîrmed frîm îthers.

I.V. Ginzburg mentiîns that new vîcabulary items in Mîdern English belîng înly tî the nîtiînal parts îf speech, i.e. înlytînîuns, verbs and adjectives; îf these nîuns are mîstnumerîus.

New vîcabulary units are as a rule mînîsemantic and mîst îf them are marked by peculiar stylistic value - they primarily belîng tî the specialised vîcabulary. Neutral wîrds and phrases are cîmparatively few. Terms used in variîus fields îf science and technique make the greater part îf new wîrds.

«Semantic wîrd-building can be divided intî shîrtening, sîund - and stress-interchange which traditiînally are referred tîminîr ways îf wîrd-fîrmatiîn. By semantic wîrd-building sîme linguists understand any change îf wîrd-meaning, e.g. stîck - the lîwer part îf the trunk îf a tree; sîmething lifeless îr stupid; the part îf an instrument that serves as a base, etc.; bench- a lîng seat îf wîîd îr stîne; a carpenter's table, etc. The majîrity îf linguists, hîwever, understand this prîcess înly as a change in the meaning îf a wîrd that may result in the appearance îf hîmînyms, as is the case with flîwer-a blîssîm and flîur-the fine meal, pîwder made frîm wheat and used fîr making bread; magazine-a publicatiîn and magazine-the chamber fîr cartridges in a gun îr rifle, etc. «The applicatiîn îf the term wîrd-fîrmatiîntî the prîcess îf semantic change and tî the appearance îf hîmînyms due tî the develîpment îf pîlysemy seems tî be debatable fîr the fîllîwing reasîns: as semantic change dîes nît, as a rule, lead tî the intrîductiîn îf a new wîrd intî the vîcabulary, it can scarcely be regarded as a wîrd-building means.

Îne îf the features îf wîrd-fîrmatiîn is an aspect îf prîductivity. All types îf wîrd-fîrmatiîn can be divided intî prîductive and nîn-prîductive. Prîductive ways are used mîre îften fîr fîrming new wîrds. Fîr instance, compounding and affixatiîn have been prîductive ways îf fîrming wîrds ever since the Îld English periîd; în the îther hand, sîund-interchange must have been at îne time a wîrd-building means but in Mîdern English its functiîn is actually înly tî distinguish between different classes and fîrms îf wîrds.

Compounds are words consisting of at least 2 stems which occur in the language as free morphemes and though they are fewer in quantity than derived or root words they still represent one of the most typical and specific features of Modern English word-structure. It should be noted that the immediate constituents of the compound word possess structural and lexical integrity. When describing the structural integrity of the compound it of great importance to examine the relations of the compound members to each other. It is believed that some compounds consist of determining and determined parts which are called determinant and the determinatum. For example in the word sunbeam the stem beam which is considered to be the basic part of the compound is the determinatum while the root sun is the determinant. The determinatum is the most essential part of the compound which undergoes inflections. What concerns to lexical intergrity it is somehow idiomatic in its character as the meaning of the whole compound is not the sum of its elements. For example the word blackboard is different from a black board as it is used as a teaching aid, besides it is not necessary that it should be black and also be a board but a piece of linoleum or other material. Or another example is the word fuss-pot which characterizes a person who is easily excited and nervous about trifles.
Compounds can express different types of relations such as place and local relations, temporal relations, functional relations etc. For instance the words suitcase, notice-board, textbook, classroom are considered to show purpose or functional relations, compounds such as sea-front, garden-party express place or local relations, temporal relations include the compound night-duty, summer-house, season-ticket. The Historical Development Of English Compounds
Compounding, which is one of the oldest methods of word-formation occurring in all Indo-European languages, is especially developed in Germanic languages. The English language has made use of compounding in all periods of its existence. Headache, heartache, rainbow, raindrop and other compounds of this type noun stem+ noun stem and its variant, such as manslaughter with the deverbal noun stem for a second element go back to Old English. To the oldest layer belong also the adjective stem+ noun stem compounds such as sweetmeat or holiday.

Some compounds preserve their type in present-day English, others have undergone phonetic changes due to which their stems ceased to be homonymous to the corresponding free forms, so that the compounds themselves were turned into root words. The phenomenon was investigated by Soviet philologists V.A. Bogoroditsky, L.A. Bulakhovsky and N.N. Armosova, who used simplification of stem. Simplification is defined as a morphological process by which a word of a complex morphological structure loses the meaning of its separate morphological parts and becomes a mere symbol of the notion given.

The English grammarians such as J.C. Nwsfield, for example used the term disguised compounds which is inconvenient because it is misleading. In the English language when a morpheme becomes the constituent of a compound, it does not affect its sound pattern. Exceptions to this rule signify therefore that the formation can not be regarded as a compound at the present stage of the language development, although it might have been the result of compounding at some earlier stage.

The degree of change can be different. Sometimes the compound is altered out of all recognition. Thus in the name of the flower daisy or in the word woman composition as the basis of the word`s origin can be discovered by etymological analysis only.

Demotivation is closely connected with simplification, but not identical with it: rather they are different aspects of changes that they may occur simultaneously. Demotivation is in fact etymological isolation when the word loses its ties with other words with which it was formerly connected and associated, ceases to be understood as belonging to its original word-family. For example kidnap [steal a child or carry off a person by illegal practice] literally means to seize a young goat. The second syllable is from an obsolete word - nap, probably closely related to nab [a slang word for arrest]. In present-day English all associations with goats or nabbing are forgotten, the word is isolated from its etymological relatives and functions as a simple sign.

The process of demotivation begins with semantic change. The change of sound form comes later. There is for some time a contradiction between meaning and form, but in the long run this contradiction is overcome as the word functions not on the strength of the meaning of the components but as a whole indivisible structure.

In many cases the two processes, the morphological and the semantic one, go hand in hand.

There are cases where one of the processes, namely, demotivation is complete, while simplification is still under way. We are inclined to rate such words as boatswain, cupboard, breakfast as compounds thanks to their conservative spelling that shows their origin, whereas in meaning and pronunciation they have changed completely and turned into simple sings for new notions. For instance breakfast originates from the verb break [interrupt] and the noun fast [going without food].

Prîductivity îf wîrd-building ways, individual derivatiînal patterns and derivatiînal affixes is understîîd as their «ability îf making new wîrds which all whî speak English find nî difficulty in understanding, in particular their ability tî create what are called îccasiînal wîrds îr nînce-wîrds. The term suggests that a speaker cîins such wîrds when he needs them; if în anîther îccasiîn the same wîrd is needed again, he cîins it afresh. Nînce-wîrds are built frîm familiar language material after familiar patterns.

Prîductivity îf derivatiînal means is relative in many respects. Mîreîver there are nîabsîlutely prîductive means; derivatiînal patterns and derivatiînal affixes pîssess different degrees îf prîductivity. Therefîre it is impîrtant that cînditiîns favîuring prîductivity and the degree îf prîductivity îf a particular pattern îr affix shîuld be established.

«Three degrees îf prîductivity are distinguished fîr affixes: I) highly-prîductive, 2) prîductive îr semi-prîductive and 3) nîn-prîductive.

Prîductive affixes are thîse used tî fîrm new wîrds in the periîd in questiîn.

Nîn-prîductive affixes are the affixes which are nît able tî fîrm new wîrds in the periîd in questiîn. Nîn-prîductive affixes are recîgnized as separate mîrphemes and pîssess clear-cut semantic characteristics.

An affix may lîîse its prîductivity and then becîme prîductive again in the prîcess îf wîrd-fîrmatiîn. This was happened tî the suffix - dîm. Fîr a lîng periîd îf time it was nîn-prîductive, but in the last years it gît a new lease îf life sî that a great amîunt îf wîrds was cîined with its help.

The prîductivity îf an affix shîuld nît be cînfused with its frequency îf îccurrence. The frequency is understîîd as the existence in the vîcabulary îf a great number îf wîrds cîntaining the affix. An affix may îccur in hundred îf wîrds, but it is nît used fîr wîrd-fîrmatiîn.

In English there are two main ways of making new words: internal and external. Internal way means updating of language vocabulary due to its internal potential. It is a productive way of word manufacturing. Internal way includes conversion, affixation, compression, abbreviation, desaffixation. External way means loanwords.

The main types of word building include inflection and derivation. Typically inflection contributes a morpheme that is required in order to ensure that the word has a form that is appropriate for the grammatical context in which it is used (tall-taller). Whereas inflection is driven by the requirement to form a word with the appropriate form in a particular grammatical context, derivation is motivated by the desire to create new lexical items using preexisting morphemes and words. When you need a new word, you do not usually need to make it up from scratch. It is possible to create new lexical items by recycling preexisting material. This is derivation. It takes one of these forms: affixation, compounding, conversion, stress placement or compounding.

Let's briefly examine them. Conversion is a way of word-formation without affix use as a result of which is formed categorically different word conterminous in some forms with initial word, e.g.: (the story was filmed).

Affixation is a way of new words formation by addition of word-formation affixes to the word stem, e.g.: (superwar, smarty, cuty, environmentalist).

Compounds are words consisting of at least 2 stems which occur in the language as free morphemes and though they are fewer in quantity than derived or root words they still represent one of the most typical and specific features of Modern English word-structure.

Desaffixation is a way of word-formation at which words are formed by rejection of a suffix or an element externally similar to a suffix, e.g.: (beggar - to beg, legislator - to legislate, burglar - to burgle).

Abbreviation means formation of new words by reduction (truncation of word stem). As a result new words are formed with the incomplete, truncated word stem (or stems), called abbreviations, e.g.: (sis - sister, prof - professor, sec - second, dif - difference).

Compression is the formation of compound words on the basis of word-combinations and sentences by decreasing the level of components of an initial word-combination or the sentence, e.g.: (do it your self - on the do-it-your-self principle; stay slim - a stay-slim diet; cat and dog - a cat and dog life).

2. Minor types of word formation

Apart from listed ways of word building English also includes minor types of word building. Minor types of word building mean non-productive means of word formation in present-day English. They include reduplication, clipping, blending, sound interchange, distinctive stress, back-formation and others.

Clipping as one of minor types of word building consists in the reduction of a word to one of its parts, e.g.:

Mathematics - maths

Laboratory - lab

Captain - cap

Gymnastics - gym

There are three types of clipping:

1. Back clipping or apocopation is the most common type, in which the beginning is retained. The unclipped original may be either a simple or a composite. Examples are: ad (advertisement), cable (cablegram), doc (doctor), exam (examination), gas (gasoline), math (mathematics), memo (memorandum), gym (gymnastics, gymnasium) mutt (muttonhead), pub (public house), pop (popular concert), trad (traditional jazz), fax (facsimile).

2. Fore-clipping or aphaeresis retains the final part. Examples are: phone (telephone), varsity (university), chute (parachute), coon (racoon), gator (alligator), pike (turnpike).

3. In middle clipping or syncope, the middle of the word is retained. Examples are: flu (influenza), tec (detective), polly (apollinaris), jams (pyjamas), shrink (head-shrinker).

4. Clipped forms are also used in compounds. One part of the original compound most often remains intact. Examples are: cablegram (cable telegram), op art (optical art), org-man (organization man), linocut (linoleum cut). Sometimes both halves of a compound are clipped as in navicert (navigation certificate). In these cases it is difficult to know whether the resultant formation should be treated as a clipping or as a blend, for the border between the two types is not always clear. According to Bauer, the easiest way to draw the distinction is to say that those forms which retain compound stress are clipped compounds, whereas those that take simple word stress are not. By this criterion bodbiz, Chicom, Comsymp, Intelsat, midcult, pro-am, sci-fi, and sitcom are all compounds made of clippings.

Accepted by the speakers of the language clipping can acquire grammatical categories (used in plural forms).

According to Marchand, clippings are not coined as words belonging to the standard vocabulary of a language. They originate as terms of a special group like schools, army, police, the medical profession, etc., in the intimacy of a milieu where a hint is sufficient to indicate the whole. For example, in school slang originated exam(ination), math(ematic), lab(oratory), and spec(ulation), tick (et = credit) originated in stock-exchange slang, whereas vet(eran), cap(tain) are army slang. While clipping terms of some influential groups can pass into common usage, becoming part of Standard English, clippings of a socially unimportant class or group will remain group slang.

Back-formation refers to the process of creating a new lexeme (less precisely, a new «word») by removing actual or supposed affixes. The resulting neologism is called a back-formation. Back-formations are shortened words created from longer words, thus back-formations may be viewed as a sub-type of clipping.

It is possible to illustrate this type of word building using an example of words beg - beggar. The word beggar was formed from the verb to beg, and on the contrary: the word borrowed from the French language beggard was formed under influence and by analogy to nouns with a suffix - er. The second syllable of the noun beggar was apprehended as a suffix, and the verb was formed by rejection of this suffix.

For example, the noun resurrection was borrowed from Latin, and the verb resurrect was then backformed hundreds of years later from it by removing the - ion suffix. This segmentation of resurrection into resurrect + ion was possible because English had many examples of Latinate words that had verb and verb+-ion pairs - in these pairs the - ion suffix is added to verb forms in order to create nouns (such as, insert/insertion, project/projection, etc.).

Back formation may be similar to the reanalyses of folk etymologies when it rests on an erroneous understanding of the morphology of the longer word. For example, the singular noun asset is a back-formation from the plural assets. However, assets is originally not a plural; it is a loan-word from Anglo-Norman asetz (modern French assez). The - s was reanalyzed as a plural suffix.

Many words came into English by this route: Pease was once a mass noun but was reinterpreted as a plural, leading to the back-formation pea. The noun statistic was likewise a back-formation from the field of study statistics. In Britain the verb burgle came into use in the 19th century as a back-formation from burglar (which can be compared to the North America verb burglarize formed by suffixation).

Even though many English words are formed this way, new coinages may sound strange, and are often used for humorous effect. For example, gruntled or pervious (from disgruntled and impervious) would be considered mistakes today, and used only in humorous contexts. The comedian George Gobel regularly used original back-formations in his humorous monologues. Bill Bryson mused that the English language would be richer if we could call a tidy-haired person shevelled - as an opposite to dishevelled.

Frequently back-formations begin in colloquial use and only gradually become accepted. For example, enthuse (from enthusiasm) is gaining popularity, though it is still considered substandard by some today.

The immense celebrations in Britain at the news of the relief of the Siege of Mafeking briefly created the verb to maffick, meaning to celebrate both extravagantly and publicly. «Maffick» was a back-formation from Mafeking, a place-name that was treated humorously as a gerund or participle.

There is a lot of different examples of back formation in English:

ablute from ablution aesthete from aesthetic

air-condition from air conditioning alm from alms

arch («to practice archery») from archery attrit from attrition

auto-destruct from auto-destruction (auto-destroy)

automate from automation bicep from biceps (non-standard)

biograph from biography blockbust from blockbuster

book-keep from book-keeping cavitate from cavitation

cherry from Old French cerise choate from inchoate

choreograph from choreography claustrophobe from claustrophobia

darkle from darkling decadent from decadence

deconstruct from deconstruction dedifferentiate from dedifferentiation

emote from emotion enthuse from enthusiasm

ept from inept escalate from escalator

eutrophicate from eutrophication extrapose from extraposition.

Back-formations of borrowed terms generally do not follow the rules of the original language. For example Homo sapiens is Latin for thinking man. As with all Linnaean species names, this is singular in Latin (plural would be hominessapientes) but it is sometimes mistakenly treated as plural in English, with the corresponding singular back-formation Homo sapien.

Some regard such divergence as incorrect, or as a mark of ignorance. Others assert that a language is determined by its usage and that strictly applying such a principle of correctness would render English a highly irregular blend of Anglo-Saxon, Latin, French and every other language from which it had ever borrowed.

Is the way îf wîrd-building: a wîrd is fîrmed by jîining twî îr mîre stems tî fîrm îne wîrd. The structural type îf cîmpîund wîrds and the wîrd-building type îfcîmpîsitiîn have certain advantages fîr cîmmunicat iîn purpîses.

Cîmpîsitiîn is nît quite sî flexible a way îf cîining new wîrds as cînversiîn but flexible enîugh. Amîng cîmpîunds are fîund numerîus expressive and cîlîurful wîrds. They are alsî cîmparatively lacînic, absîrbing intî îne wîrd an idea that îtherwise wîuld have required a whîle phrase (cf. Thehîtel was full îf week-enders and The hîtel was full îf peîple spending the week-end there).

Bîth the lacînic and the expressive value îf cîmpîunds can be well illustrated by English cîmpîund adjectives denîting cîlîurs (cf. snîw-white - as white as snîw).

There are twî characteristic features îf English cîmpîunds:

a) Bîth cîmpînents in an English cîmpîund are free stems, that is they can be used as wîrds with a distinctive meaning îf their îwn. The sîund pattern will be the same except fîr the stresses, e.g. «a green-hîuse» and «a green hîuse».

b) English cîmpîunds have a twî-stem pattern, with the exceptiîn îf cîmpîund wîrds which have fîrm-wîrd stems in their structure, e.g.middle-îf-the-rîad, îff-the-recîrd, up-and-dîing etc.

Edward Sapir observed that nothing is more natural than the prevalence of reduplication - the repetition of the base of a word in part or in its entirety. He observed that, though rare, reduplication is found in English, e.g.:

pooh-pooh goody-goody wishy-washy

sing-song roly-poly harum-scarum

Later on, Thun showed that reduplication is less marginal than is commonly assumed. He listed and examined about 2,000 reduplicative words in standard English and in various dialects.

Reduplicatives (compound words formed by reduplication) are different. The most significant property of these words is that word-formation is driven by phonological factors.

There are two main types of reduplicatives: rhyme motivated compounds and ablaut motivated compounds. Rhyme here means what it means in poetry: the vowels and any consonant(s) that appear after it in the last syllable are identical, while ablaut means a change in the root vowel. Usually ablaut signals a change in grammatical function, e.g. the o e alternation in long (adj.) vs. length (noun) marks a difference in word-class. These labels for the two categories of reduplicative compounds highlight the fact that the repetition of the bases in compounds of this kind involves copying the rhyme in so-called rhyme motivated compounds, and coping the consonants and altering the vowel in ablaut motivated compounds.

Some rhyming compounds are formed by joining bases which are both pre-existing words as in Black-Jack and brain-drain. Probably more common, however, are rhyming compounds where one (or both) bases is not an independent word, as in:

Rhyme motivated compounds:

nitwit helter-skelter namby-pamby

titbit hobnob higgledy-piggledy

nitty-gritty teeny-weeny hurly-burly

Finally, there are ablaut motivated compounds in which one or both bases may not be an independent word:

Ablaut motivated compounds:

tip-top riff-raff ding-dong shilly-shally

tick-tock tittle-tattle wibble-wobble dingle-dangle

ping-pong dilly-dally flip-flop mish-mash

Sound interchange is the way of word building when some sounds are changed to form a new word. It is non-productive in Modern English; it was productive in Old English and can be met in other Indo-European languages.

The causes of sound interchange can be different. It can be the result of Ancient Ablaut which cannot be explained by the phonetic laws during the period of the language development known to scientists, e.g. to strike - stroke, to sing - song etc. It can be also the result of Ancient Umlaut or vowel mutation which is the result of palatalizing the root vowel because of the front vowel in the syllable coming after the root (regressive assimilation), e.g. hot - to heat (hotian), blood - to bleed (blodian) etc.

In many cases we have vowel and consonant interchange. In nouns we have voiceless consonants and in verbs we have corresponding voiced consonants because in Old English these consonants in nouns were at the end of the word and in verbs in the intervocal position, e.g. bath - to bathe, life - to live, breath - to breathe etc.

Stress interchange can be mostly met in verbs and nouns of Romanic origin: nouns have the stress on the first syllable and verbs on the last syllable, e.g. `accent - to ac`cent. This phenomenon is explained in the following way: French verbs and nouns had different structure when they were borrowed into English; verbs had one syllable more than the corresponding nouns. When these borrowings were assimilated in English the stress in them was shifted to the previous syllable (the second from the end). Later on the last unstressed syllable in verbs borrowed from French was dropped (the same as in native verbs) and after that the stress in verbs was on the last syllable while in nouns it was on the first syllable. As a result of it we have such pairs in English as: to af`fix -`affix, to con`flict - `conflict, to ex`port -`export, to ex`tract - `extract etc. As a result of stress interchange we have also vowel interchange in such words because vowels are pronounced differently in stressed and unstressed positions.

It is the way of word building when imitating different sounds forms a word. There are some semantic groups of words formed by means of sound imitation:

a) Sounds produced by human beings, such as: to whisper, to giggle, to mumble, to sneeze, to whistle etc.

b) Sounds produced by animals, birds, insects, such as: to hiss, to buzz, to bark, to moo, to twitter etc.

c) Sounds produced by nature and objects, such as: to splash, to rustle, to clatter, to bubble, to ding-dong, to tinkle etc.

The corresponding nouns are formed by means of conversion, e.g. clang (of a bell), chatter (of children) etc.

There are three cases of such variations:

1. Modifications of the pronunciation of a word depending on the context or its position. Utterance and the conjunction are reduced in these combinations.

now and then

King and Queen

The conjunction is reduced in these combinations.

2. Accentual variation - different coexisting stress patterns of one and the same word.

Br. territory Am. territory

Br. dictionary Am. dictionary

3. Emic variation - multiple pronunciation of one and the same word.

explain [i] [e]

begin [i] [e]

ceramic [si`ramic] [ki`ramic]

drastic [a:] [a]

Morphological variation takes place when different derivational morphemes are used without changing the word's meaning.

Academic, academicals

Morphologic, morphological

But not: historic and historical - they are not morphological variants, but synonyms. Historic - memorable in history associated with past time:

a historic event.

Historical - belonging to history (real not imaginative) or dealing with real events in history:

a historical novel

historical events (real events)

This is a historic and historical place.

Lexical variations are determined by different registers, e.g.:

formal / informal

spoken / written

laboratory / lab

examination / exam

television / tele

often / oft (poetic)

Semantic variations

The majority of words in any language have more than one meaning.

Topology (philological term) - the term from the domain of mathematics, refers to the study of continuity and variability, invariant and its variants, identity and differentiation. Lexicology studies the problems of synonymy, polysemy, homonymy, identity-of-unit problem. The key point here is to try and strike the balance in interaction of the invariant and the variants, always remembering that in philology it is invariant that comes first focuses on different types of variation in lexis.

Allo-emic theory - treatment of all elements in language as being sets of variants ('allo'-terms) of some invariants ('eme'-terms). Invariants are morphemes, phonemes, lexemes, which belong to the systemic level (language) while allomorphs, allophones, allolexes are their positional variants respectively and belong to the level of actualization (speech).

Allonymic variation - realized in contextual pairs semantically co-ordinated like slow and careful; quick and impatient.

'Emic' variation - a) a type of phonetic variation which occurs when there are multiple pronunciations for a single word: begin [bi'gin], [b 'gin]; explain [ik'splein], [ek'splein]; direct [dai'rekt], [di'rekt]; b) morphological variation with allomorphs of the same morpheme involved: irregular, innavigable, immovable, illegal.

Morphological theory provides the tools for analyzing `real' words like shopkeeper and conversations which are listed in dictionaries and which probably most competent, adult speakers of English know. But, if it stopped at that, it would be failing in its task of characterizing the nature of speakers' lexical knowledge. The true English vocabulary goes far beyond the institutionalized words listed in dictionaries. Obviously, a very considerable number of words must simply be memorized, e.g. words made up of a single morpheme - zebra, tree, saddle - there is no way one can work out their meaning. Word formation can be faddish. A word, especially one that captures the spirit of the times, may spawn numerous imitations. Take the 1980s word yuppie, which was formed by adding the suffix spelled as - y or - ie to the initial letters of either `Young Urban Professional Person' or `Young Upwardly Mobile Professional Person'. It spawned imitations like yuppify, yuppidom, yuppette, buppie (`black yuppie'), guppie (`gay yuppie'), etc.

Many of the nonce, non-institionalised words are compounds. If a speaker wants to express an idea which would normally be expressed by a syntactic phrase in a manner that heightens its concreteness and salience, it is possible as a one-off, hyphenated compound. The newspaper columnist Melanie Philips manufactured the word `anything-goes-as-long-as-you-can-get-away-with-it-culture' which is an excellent example of this phenomenon:

`Public life has fallen into disrepute and the cynicism of the people knows no bounds. It's the anything-goes-as-long-as-you-can-get-away-with-it-culture, and it is a prevalent in the corridors of Whitehall as in the joyriders' ghettos'.

At the other end of the spectrum old words go out of use, e.g. wone meaning `home, abode' is now obsolete. We can see that wone is opsolete while porret survives in the dialectal use but it is very rare. The line between `dialectal and very rare' and `obsolete' is a fine one.

3. Blending as a type of modern English word formation

Blending is another productive word-formation process, which is a combination of two or more words to create a new one, usually by taking the beginning of the other word and the end of the other one. So new words are created.

Sometimes blending is referred to as portmanteau words. The term portmanteau was coined by Lewis Carroll in 1882, when in his book Through the Looking Glass Humpty Dumpty describes a new word he uses as follows: «Well, `slithy' means `lithe and slimy'. It's like a Portmanteau - there are two meanings packed into one word» (Carroll 1996,102 - i.e. there are two different words with completely unequal meanings put together to form a new word with a new meaning.

Blending has been investigated in a variety of studies: In one of the earliest studies, Pound (1914: 1) analyzes 314 blends, proposing the following definition:

Blend-words may be defined as two or more words, often of cognate sense, telescoped as it were into one; as factitious conflations which retain, for a while at least, the suggestive power of their various elements.

She argues that blends have to be distinguished from (among other things) - analogical extensions or enlargements (such as judgmatical [judgment-dogmatical]) because (i) judgmatical does not imply the meaning of dogmatical and, thus, no semantic fusion has occurred and(ii) such forms are ` `generally unintentional'' whereas blends are ` `often conscious or intentional'' however, on the same page, she acknowledges that neither criterion is failsafe;

- whimsical folk-etymological perversions (such as jawbacious [jaw - audacious]) because of their folk-etymological origin - again, however, Pound admits that ` `the subjects of folk-etymology and blending do merge. The test of motive in origin is not always either a clear nor a trustworthy guide''

- agglutinative or elliptical forms or contractions of frequently cooccurring

expressions (such as starkarageous [stark - outrageous]) because the ` `predominant motive in their formation was clearly elliptical''. There are some problems with this distinction: first, while Pound does not count them as blends, she nevertheless says ` `[t] hese[contractions] are undoubtedly blends'' but does not provide a motive for blend creation according to which ` `real'' blends and her contractions can be distinguished. Second, some expressions she considers contractions are definitely not blends in any sense: Frisco (from San Francisco), for example, does not involve the fusion of elements of two words at all. Finally, as before, Pound claims that in some cases the distinction is not an absolute one.

Algeo's definition of blends is similar to the one I proposed above: Blending refers to ` `a combination of two or more forms, at least one of which has been shortened in the process of combination.'' This definition is based on structural characteristics and implies that, for example, cases where full forms combine without overlap do not count as blends but rather as compounds; examples of non-blends mentioned include squandermania, daisy (historically a compound, namely day's eye) and meritocracy (` `a derivative with the combining form - ocracy'). However, I believe the case of meritocracy is a difficult one since, strictly speaking, meritocracy can be argued to be covered by Algeo's definition of blends (merit - aristocracy), so it seems as if the definition is either not followed by consistently or is in need of refinement in terms of additional criteria.1 Additionally, he also points out some cases where the dividing line between blends and other derivational processes is far from clear: for instance, while breadth can be analyzed asa blend (OE brede-length), it is equally plausible an instance of analogical lextension following the pattern long - length: broad - x. Also, he argues that in cases like dumbfound (dumb - confound) blending may be difficult to distinguish from what he calls free composition.

Cannon's (1986) paper is based on an analysis of 132 written English blends. After a thorough review of the literature, he formulates a definition which also gives some criteria that are, although not necessary or sufficient, characteristic of the most typical blends:

A blend involves a telescoping of two or more separate forms into one, or, rarely, a superposition of one form upon another. It usually contains overlapping and preserves some of the meaning of at least one of the source words, though sometimes so much of the roots are lost that a blend is unanalyzable.

However, little explicit discussion of how blends differ from superficially similar phenomena can be found. For instance, Cannon does not address the question raised by Algeo (1977) and Pound (1914) whether forms like radarange (what Pound and Algeo would have called a contraction) do constitute blends or not.

Bauer's definition of blends is ` `[a] blend may be defined as a new lexeme formed from parts of two (or possibly more) other words in such a way that there is no transparent analysis into morphs, '' but already the following sentence questions his own definition by (correctly) pointing out that ` `in many cases some kind of analysis can be made[because] at least one of the elements is transparently recoverable.'' Later on, he adds that ` `blends normally take the first part of one word and the last part of another''. As to distinguishing blends from othe rderivational processes, he points out cases where one source word is left intact in the blend, which might therefore be analyzed as the addition of one source word to a case of clipping (examples include mocamp [motor - camp] and Amtrack [American - track]), but he does not seem to take a definite stand on how to resolve the issue. It is hard to see, however, how mocampfits into, for example, his own traditional definition of compounds since mo is not a word or a free morpheme.

Stekauer (1991) is a typical example of the classificatory approach towards blends. His definition is, strictly speaking, slightly circular: blends ` `have resulted from two motivating words which have been blended into a new coinage which is unanalysable into determinant and determinatum, thus representing monemes''. Like others before him, he points to the importance of phonemic overlap in distinguishing blends from compounds and, following Pound (1914), contends that elliptical forms (such as trafficator [traffic - indicator]) are not blends as they do not constitute a new meaning resulting from the blending process.

Finally, let us turn to Kemmer (2003), who adopts Bauer's definition of blends: ` `a new lexeme formed from parts of two or more other lexemes.'' Like others, she comments on the role played by phonemic overlap and phonemic as well as phonological similarity, correctly emphasizing that these properties are not necessary conditions for lexical blends. She summarizes as follows:

Blends combine parts of lexical source words, rather than whole source words; this distinguishes them from compounds. Morphological structure is not particularly relevant to blends. Phonological properties are highly relevant to blending; phonological similarity of the blend with part or whole source words increases the likelihood or felicity of the blend.

This brief characterization of previous accounts of the distinction between blends and related/similar products of word-formation processes highlights the most important features figuring in the definition of blends; for a more thorough overview, the reader is referred to the comprehensive survey by Cannon (1986).

Cannon (1986) proposes criteria similar to those of Algeo (e.g. by looking at the overlap of source words in blends and the location of the point of fusion), but includes some more parameters such as word classes, syllabic lengths, and morphological properties of the source words, semantic groups of the denotata of blends, etc. Simply speaking, all possibly relevant information is catalogued, 2 but when it comes to theoretical conclusions bearing on the structure of blends Cannon appears a bit indecisive. On the one hand, he correctly notes that ` `a blend should notdi.er very much in form and meaning from its sources'' and ` `the major parts of the source words should be preserved'' (1986) - on the other hand, he points out that ``our blends are little illuminated by ananalysis of sound, phonotactics, and the tiny bit of rhyme. Their segments are too varied to suggest any propensities for blending'' (1986).

Bauer (1983) is concerned with by now already familiar distinctions.

He mainly differentiates between (i) blends where only parts of the original words figure in the coinage, for example, chunnel (channel _ tunnel), and (ii) blends where the two words used as the bases are both present in their entirety, for example, glasphalt (glass _ asphalt), involving overlapin pronunciation, spelling or both.3 An additional group is discussed, namely that where the blend looks as if it is ` `analysable in terms of other word-formation processes, in particular as a neo-classical compound'' (1983), for example, autocide (automobile _ suicide).

S ¡tekauer (1991) merely proposes an ` `onomasiological'' classification of blends (arguing for an improvement over purely formal classifications) and discusses various individual examples; his conclusions, however, do not seem to go beyond previous research. Finally, let us turn to Kemmer (2003). She introduces a terminological distinction between intercalative blends (` `in which the two words in the blend are so tightly integrated […] that the sounds of one source word are interspersed between the other'' (2003), for example, chortle [chuckle _ snort]) and nonintercalative or sequential blends. There are two problems with this distinction: on the one hand, Kemmer states that` `[t] here are no intercalative blends in my data that do not also have a possible non-intercalative analysis'' (2003), which, if true, raises the question of the explanatory value of this distinction (cf. Occam's razor).On the other hand, Kemmer undermines her own distinction by citing examples which are in fact intercalative without having a linear analysis, namelychortle and slithy (slimy _ lithe).

4. The classification of blends

The beginning of one word is added to the end of the other. For example, brunch is a blend of breakfast and lunch. One of the two may be a whole word if it is short. This is the most common method of blending. A monosyllabic word is divided into its onset and rime if necessary. A blend of this type typically has the same number of syllables as the second word.

broccoli + cauliflower - broccoflower

breakfast + lunch - brunch

camera + recorder - camcorder

education + entertainment - edutainment

information + commercial - infomercial

motor + hotel - motel

simultaneous + broadcast - simulcast


Ïîäîáíûå äîêóìåíòû

  • Main types of word formation: inflection and derivation. Types of clipping, unclipped original. Blending, back-formation and reduplication. Sound and stress interchange. Phonetic, morphological, lexical variations. Listing and institutionalization.

    êîíòðîëüíàÿ ðàáîòà [24,3 K], äîáàâëåí 30.12.2011

  • How important is vocabulary. How are words selected. Conveying the meaning. Presenting vocabulary. How to illustrate meaning. Decision - making tasks. Teaching word formation and word combination. Teaching lexical chunks. Teaching phrasal verbs.

    äèïëîìíàÿ ðàáîòà [2,4 M], äîáàâëåí 05.06.2010

  • Word-building as one of the main ways of enriching vocabulary and the affixation is one of the most productive ways. Studying of affixation, which play important role in word-formation, classifying of affixes according to its structure and semantics.

    äèïëîìíàÿ ðàáîòà [62,2 K], äîáàâëåí 21.07.2009

  • The structure of words and word-building. The semantic structure of words, synonyms, antonyms, homonyms. Word combinations and phraseology in modern English and Ukrainian languages. The Native Element, Borrowed Words, characteristics of the vocabulary.

    êóðñ ëåêöèé [95,2 K], äîáàâëåí 05.12.2010

  • The nature of English word stress - the key to excellent pronunciation and understanding of English. English speakers use word stress to communicate rapidly and accurately, even in difficult conditions. Word stress tendencies and functions, variation.

    ðåôåðàò [22,6 K], äîáàâëåí 06.02.2010

  • Shortening of spoken words. Graphical abbreviations and acronyms. Abbreviations as the major type of shortenings. Secondary ways of shortening: sound interchange and sound imitating. Blendening of words. Back formation as a source for shortening of words.

    äèïëîìíàÿ ðàáîòà [90,2 K], äîáàâëåí 10.07.2009

  • Loan-words of English origin in Russian Language. Original Russian vocabulary. Borrowings in Russian language, assimilation of new words, stresses in loan-words. Loan words in English language. Periods of Russian words penetration into English language.

    êóðñîâàÿ ðàáîòà [55,4 K], äîáàâëåí 16.04.2011

  • General guidelines on word stress: one word has only one stress; stress vowels, not consonants. Origins of the word stress and the notion of accent. English accentuation tendencies. Typical patterns of stress of nouns, verbs, adjectives and adverbs.

    êóðñîâàÿ ðàáîòà [275,8 K], äîáàâëåí 12.04.2014

  • The description of neologisms: definition, diachronic analysis, cultural acceptance factor. The manor and major word building types, presents latest top 50 neologisms, analyzed and arranged in table according to their word building type, sphere of usage.

    êóðñîâàÿ ðàáîòà [43,5 K], äîáàâëåí 19.04.2011

  • Modern English vocabulary from the point of view of its etymology (origin) may be divided into 3 great groups. Words belonging to the set of native word-stock are for the most part. Periods of French borrowings. Assimilation of borrowings and their types.

    ïðåçåíòàöèÿ [41,4 K], äîáàâëåí 20.10.2013

Ðàáîòû â àðõèâàõ êðàñèâî îôîðìëåíû ñîãëàñíî òðåáîâàíèÿì ÂÓÇîâ è ñîäåðæàò ðèñóíêè, äèàãðàììû, ôîðìóëû è ò.ä.
PPT, PPTX è PDF-ôàéëû ïðåäñòàâëåíû òîëüêî â àðõèâàõ.
Ðåêîìåíäóåì ñêà÷àòü ðàáîòó.